Dear Tin,
I’m a hopeless romantic and I’ve just finished watching
the movie adaptation of Nicholas Sparks’ Dear John. No, I’m not going to talk
about romance or anything war-related. But there were two lines which struck me
as I was watching the film, and the conversation went this way,
Savannah: Did you ever notice how big the
moon is when it’s rising, and how little it is when it’s up in the sky?
John: Yeah,
but it’s only perspective. It doesn’t matter where it is in the sky, or where
you are in the world… it’s never bigger than your thumb.
Yup, he’s probably right but I still can’t understand
why. Can you help me?
Senserely yours,
Thinking-of-the-moon
Hello there! As I am
writing to you now, I can’t help but also notice how the moon seems to be far
up in the sky (and yes, my thumb can also cover it when I stretch my arm). Just
like you, I have also wondered how the moon plays these tricks until I came
across certain theories which answered the questions I had in mind. Savannah’s question
could be explained by elaborating on the moon
illusion while John’s reply could be understood further by introducing the
concept of visual angles--- which is
also somehow related to the moon illusion, as we would see later on.
The phenomenon when the
horizon moon seems to be larger than the elevated moon is known as the moon
illusion (Goldstein, 2010). Rock and Kaufman (1962) rejects the eye-elevation hypothesis proposed by
Boring and his colleagues. According to this hypothesis, the horizon moon
appears larger since it is viewed directly at eye level while the zenith moon appears
smaller since the observer must look up. Rock and Kaufman (1962) offers an alternative
view in which they tested the effect of the absence or presence of terrain
through the apparent distance theory. The
horizon moon is accompanied by a terrain which involves a number of depth cues
as opposed to the overhead moon which has no terrain. Thus, this creates a perception of greater
distance for the horizon moon. To further understand this, let’s bring in the
concept of visual angles. Visual angle
refers to the angle created by an object relative to the observer’s eye, and it
depends on both the stimulus size and the distance from the observer
(Goldstein, 2010). Thus, although the moon’s size and distance remains constant
as viewed here on Earth, there is an apparent
change in distance that results from the additional depth information
provided by the terrain. This is why the horizon moon seems farther than the
overhead moon. Despite this, observers may not be consciously aware that they are perceiving the horizon moon with a greater distance. Some even report that the horizon moon is nearer, and this may be due to the concept of relative size (Rock & Kaufman, 1962). Relative size states that when two objects are of the same size, the one that is farther away takes up less of the visual field than the closer one (Goldstein, 2010). Thus, observers may reason out that the horizon moon is nearer since it appears to be bigger, but this is not what the apparent distance theory seeks to explain.
Rock and Kaufman (1962)
claim that the terrain is crucial in forming the moon illusion, and the varying
magnitude of the illusion is influenced by terrain and sky conditions. They
also found that obstructing the observer’s view of the terrain leads to the disappearance
of the illusion and inverting the terrain that comes with the horizon moon
lessens the said illusion. Moreover, they acknowledged that another factor might
play a role and cited the effect of framing or relational effect which is the
case when the horizon moon is viewed between buildings or other land objects.
This brings us to the next theory, the angular
size contrast theory which states that surrounding the moon with larger objects
makes it appear smaller, as what happens when the overhead moon is surrounded
by the whole stretch of the sky (Goldstein, 2010). According to Plug and Ross (1994),
there is an increase in the perceived angular size of horizon objects by about a
factor of 1.5-2.0 in diameter. No wonder that the moon seems bigger when viewed in the horizon. This illusion is likewise affected by scene details. (Note that we keep on using terms like apparent and seem because we are analyzing things based on how perceive them and not on what is actually out there)
Now that we have
addressed your first concern, we move on to the next one. John said that wherever
you are in the world, the moon would never be bigger than your thumb. How could this
be when we know for a fact that the moon has a diameter of about 2200 miles as
compared to the very small width of your thumb? Literally speaking, the moon is
way much bigger than our thumb, but if we put things into perspective, what
John said was right. Considering that the moon is 245,000 miles away from
Earth, the resulting visual angle (which we talked about earlier) becomes very small, and measures at around 0.5°. The thumb held at arm’s length, on the
other hand, compensates for its small size with its much closer distance to the eye. Thus, it has a visual angle of 2° when held at arm’s length. Since the moon’s visual angle is
only ¼ of that of our thumb, we do not have trouble covering the moon.
It's amazing, right? I hope you got something from here. If you want to explore more about visual angles, why not try covering the moon with your pinky finger? PS: Tell me if it also works that way. :D
It's amazing, right? I hope you got something from here. If you want to explore more about visual angles, why not try covering the moon with your pinky finger? PS: Tell me if it also works that way. :D
Senserely yours,
Tin
References:
Dear John. [Photograph]. Retrieved on February 5, 2014, from: http://wakeups.livejournal.com/868.html
Goldstein, E.B. (2010). Sensation and Perception. Belmont, California: Wadsworth Cengage Learning
Rock, I., & Kaufman, L. (1962). The moon illusion, II. Science, 136(3521), 1023-1031.
Plug, C., & Ross, H. E. (1994). The natural moon illusion: a multifactor angular account. Perception, 23, 321-333.
No comments:
Post a Comment